Recent Events

Freeman invented a new sub-genre, the two-part inverted story , which shows the criminal committing the crime, and then how the detective solves it. Some believed that detective fiction should be more naturalistic. Tellingly, one of A.

Bentley turned his detective from a superior reasoning machine into a fallible human being, capable of falling in love or getting the solution wrong. Chesterton and H. Bailey late s used the detective story as a vehicle for ideas; they commented on social issues and criticized philosophies and ideologies. Only the detective has the insight, and imaginative sympathy, to understand the true significance of events. Many are social satires that show how big business, politics, and the law exploit the ordinary working man.

Others are powerful studies of morbid psychology. While Christie achieved a remarkable success in with The Murder of Roger Ackroyd , she did not come into her kingdom as Queen of Crime until the mids. Other members of the Freeman-Crofts school included G. Cole , John Rhode , J. Connington, and Henry Wade both These writers dominated British detective fiction well into the s.

Van Dine , an art critic who took up mystery writing while recovering from a nervous breakdown, invented the puzzle plot.

The Eyes of The Chameleon: A Berkeley Mystery by Graham Dixon (Paperback) - Lulu

This is what most people think of as a detective story: Van Dine argued that the detective story was an elaborate puzzle, but that, instead of emphasizing detection as in Freeman and Crofts , the form should emphasize fair play and a battle of wits. Sayers revolted against the dominance of the pure detection school. They believed the detective story should be more literary, and more concerned with character. The pure form fell into neglect, as the Van Dinean puzzle plot, the Sayersian comedy of manners, and the Ilesian crime study dominated.

Some writers in the Freeman-Crofts school, particularly Henry Wade, developed more interest in characterization. Agatha Christie really hit her stride, with a series of ingenious mysteries starring Hercule Poirot, the diminutive Belgian with the egg-shaped head and little grey cells. Once WWII broke out, her stories deepened, and she became increasingly concerned with how murder affected the innocent, and with the problem of evil. Many of the younger writers looked to Christie as an example of how the detective story should be structured.

Day-Lewis were staunch traditionalists in form, who humanized the detective story. Nigel Strangeways, his series detective, is one of the most likeable sleuths in the genre. John Dickson Carr alias Carter Dickson was one of the few writers to equal, if not surpass, Christie.

He was the master of the impossible crime — the victim stabbed in a room locked and bolted on the inside, or strangled on a muddy beach without a footprint in sight; the murderer who vanishes into thin air. He was a terrific storyteller: A lover of adventure in the grand manner, and increasingly dissatisfied with the post-war world, his later books were historical novels, many set in the Restoration or the Regency.

Margery Allingham never wrote the same book twice, while her detective, Albert Campion, is something of a chameleon. Her admirers praise her for her characterization, style, and moral concerns, but her plots are eccentric, and sometimes hard to grasp. Gladys Mitchell was one of the Big Three women detective writers of the s, while her detective, the reptilian, witch-like psychiatrist Mrs.

Bradley, has often been called the best woman detective in fiction. Michael Innes was the donnish detective writer nonpareil, founder of a school whose books are fantastical and abound in literary allusions. He wrote half a dozen brilliantly rich and imaginative works, and then churned out a long series of artificial, formulaic, self-indulgent books in which style is an end in itself. His early books, though, are stunning. Edmund Crispin , who took his name from an Innes character, is a delight. The books are high-spirited, burst with good humor and invention, and the plots are ingenious, if gimmicky.

The detective story hit a low point for several decades after WWII. The genre seemed played out. Many writers continued to produce detective stories, but the crime novel became the main form. Julian Symons and H. Keating claimed that the crime novel was not merely different, but vastly superior to, the detective story. Some writers such as P. James continued to write detective novels in the s manner of Blake, albeit with a more serious tone, and at much greater length. Ruth Rendell alternated between cleverly plotted mysteries featuring Inspector Wexford, and often squalid non-series crime novels.

Of late, earnest social commentary loomed too large. Others, like Colin Dexter , creator of Inspector Morse, wrote brilliant formal detective stories, with multi-faceted clues and multiple solutions, often hinging on unbreakable alibis or impersonations.

Chameleon EYES!

Light comes in the eye from the top and the rods and cone photocells are at the bottom, just above a special layer at the very bottom called the retinal pigment epithelium RPE. The photocells carry out some sophisticated chemistry in which certain chemicals change form in response to light. Proteins from the photocells change shape cis to trans isomers and this causes them to change from being pigmented to transparent.

This means that the chemicals have to be recycled and it must be done quickly, otherwise we would not be able to detect rapid light changes and our eyes would get tired quickly. The retina and optic nerve are essentially extensions of the brain and the retina itself does very sophisticated processing that would be equivalent to a very powerful computer. In in Byte magazine there was an interesting statement from a professor of physiology and biomedial engineering considered the processing done by the nerves in the retina compared to what a Cray supercomputer of the time was able to do: For details see http: Keeping in mind there are 10 million or more such cells interacting with each other in complex ways, it would take a minimum of a hundred years of Cray time to simulate what takes place in your eye many times every second.

But why the inverted design of the retina? First, the cells in the back of the eye are transparent to a high degree. In addition, certain cells in front of the photocells act like organic optical fibers, transmitting light through the several layers to the photoreceptors. To see an interesting photo of this, go to this web article by creationist Dr.

Jonathan Sarfati click here. This is pretty recent research published in by researchers from Leipzig University in Germany. There are cells called Muller cells that are shaped like a funnel near the inner surface of the eye but like an optical fiber as they transmit light through the retina layers. The Muller cells also have the correct index of refraction to transmit the light with minimum loss and distortion. Even without the amazing Muller cells, there would be little attenuation of the light because of the transparent nature of these layers.

For the same reason it does not make that much difference which direction the photocells point. This fiber optic type design makes the eye very efficient in its use of the light. But, something that does matter very critically is that the photocells must have a very abundant blood supply that will allow toxins to be carried away and used chemicals to be recycled quickly. Thus, for the RPE to be at the bottom of the retina immediately behind the photocells makes sense in order to provide a good blood supply and to carry away heat. The RPE has complex functions that the eye depends on.

The retina of some organisms is not inverted, such as the squid for example. However, squid eyes are significantly inferior to human eyes, but are fine for what squids need under water. The inverted retina of vertebrates is not a poor design or an accident. It has the characteristics of an intelligently engineered system. This implies intelligent design because of the way that multiple systems have to be coordinated to work together. Evolutionists often talk about things coming about "by chance and necessity. Chance cannot generate complex information; neither can it coordinate multiple systems to operate together.

This coordination requires intelligent planning and design. There are many things about our sight that just works effortlessly for most of us, unless we have a problem with it. Humans are not like chameleons, whose eyes are able to move independently of each other, allowing them to look at two different objects at once. Our eyes are designed to focus and move together. Our eyes always work to focus on the same object.

Each of our eyes send a set of signals to the brain but in the brain the signals from the left and right eyes are mapped to one image. There are small differences between the information coming from the two eyes, and this information is used in depth perception. Recent research at Berkeley has shown that the retina separates what we see into 10 or 12 separated video streams that are processed in the brain as layers.

How the brain creates our perception of what we see from this is a mystery. Click to read about this. Our eyes are able to adjust their angle to direct our focus, such as when we are watching a moving object. The fovea is an area in the back of the eye the light focuses onto where there are the most photocells and where we have the clearest vision.

So if we see something in the corner of our eye using our peripheral vision, we tend to turn our eyes and then turn our head to bring that object in clearest view. There are six muscles outside the eyeball that are responsible for moving the eye in all the various three dimensional angles our eyes can turn. These muscles work in combination to provide precise control of the rotations of the eyeball. When one muscle on one side of the eye contracts, the opposing muscle or muscles relax. The eye muscles never totally relax however, they are always exerting at least some tension and movement.

Even when you are fixing your gaze at something in a steady fashion, your eyes are carrying out many minute rapid movements back and forth. These are sometimes called fixational eye movements. There is debate over the reason for these movements. These movements apparently relate to the fact that the retina temporarily stops processing light sometimes, such as while the eye is rapidly turning or when a bright light shines in the eye. If the eyes did not undergo small movements when you look steady at a stationary object, the retina might stop processing and quickly become unable to detect changes in what you see.

The eye muscles are composed of two types of muscle fibers. One set are for the larger voluntary movements such as when you are tracking an object with your eyes. The other muscle fibers are for the involuntary small fixation movements when you fix your gaze on an object. For the two eyes to keep focus on the same object, the brain must control the six muscles around each eye so the eyes move correctly together. Controlling the eye movements is a complex process involving feedback between the eyes, nerves, brain, and muscles.

Some types of movements are compensated for by the brain, such as when you tilt your head while looking at an upright object. While tilting your head, the brain's image of what you're seeing stays upright so you don't get dizzy or disoriented too easily.

The Eyes of The Chameleon: A Berkeley Mystery

There are other designs in the way the eyes adjust and compensate for various effects. While eye movements are tracking a moving object, the eye may have to adjust for distance to keep focused on the object as its distance to the eye changes. The inner ear is also tied into the visual system to aid in sensing direction and visual focus while the head is turning or the body is moving. All this is interpreted and managed by calculations in the brain. Adjusting for distance in focusing on objects is done by the eye adjusting the shape of the lens.

Special fibers around the lens pull on it to make the lens thinner for focus at longer distances and the pull is relaxed to make the lens thicker for focusing at smaller distances. There has been some research on how the eye can do some self-correction of optical imperfections in the cornea and the lens.

The cornea is the clear bulging area in the front of the eyeball which has the iris within it for admitting various amounts of light. Imperfections in the shape of the cornea or the lens cause various vision issues if they are significant. Even someone with very good vision has some imperfections in their eyes. I will use my own eyes and glasses as an example of how the eye can adapt. I think it is an argument for design that I am able to adapt to my eye condition. I have a condition known as Amblyopia, also known as lazy eye. My left eye never totally focuses properly and does not work with the right eye properly.

As a result my brain tends to emphasize my right eye in interpreting what I see. I don't see blur unless I close my right eye.


  • Engineers create chameleon-like artificial 'skin' that shifts color on demand.
  • symmetry magazine | dimensions of particle physics.
  • Engineers create chameleon-like artificial 'skin' that shifts color on demand.
  • Anne Rices Unauthorized French Quarter Tour: That Other Tour Presents (Vampire Chronicles and Mayfair Witches Book 1).
  • The Sponsorship Handbook: Essential Tools, Tips and Techniques for Sponsors and Sponsorship Seekers;
  • Tag: anthony berkeley;
  • Nuer Primer One: Bok Kel.

But even with my left eye as it is and without my glasses on, I can pass a drivers eye test. I can pass the eye test in spite of seeing blur, because I have learned to interpret what I see.

Post navigation

For children with lazy eye, some opthalmalogists have them do a series of eye exercizes that are designed to force the use of the weak eye so the brain does not ignore it as much. I tried this for a while as a young person. It may have helped me interpret what I see better but I don't think it really changed my eyes. Today, I wear glasses that are progressive trifocals. This means that the two sides of my glasses are different from each other, and each side of my glasses have regions with three different focal lengths.

So my eyes must adjust the left eye differently than the right eye so that the two eyes focus on an object. Also, when I change between the different focal areas of my glasses, the two eyes must each adjust to a new focal length. Some people have difficulty getting used to bifocals or trifocals. Other than up close reading I generally like my trifocal glasses. However I have another pair made just for up close work like reading. It is amazing that I can get along as well as I do, considering that my left eye sees things in a blurry way all the time.

All this is a testimony to how God has designed the eye as an adaptable instrument, so that it can adapt to a potential problem that could arise.